翻訳と辞書
Words near each other
・ Hyper-real Religion
・ Hyper-threading
・ Hyper-V
・ Hypera zoilus
・ Hyperacanthus
・ Hyperacanthus (plant)
・ HyperACCESS
・ Hyperaccumulator
・ Hyperaccumulators table – 3
・ Hyperacrius
・ Hyperactivation
・ Hyperactive (disambiguation)
・ Hyperactive!
・ Hyperacuity
・ Hyperacuity (album)
Hyperacuity (scientific term)
・ Hyperacusis
・ Hyperaemia
・ Hyperaeschra
・ Hyperaeschrella
・ Hyperaeschrella nigribasis
・ Hyperaldosteronism
・ Hyperalgesia
・ Hyperallergic
・ Hyperalonia
・ Hyperalonia morio
・ Hyperaminoacidemia
・ Hyperammonemia
・ Hyperamoeba
・ Hyperandra


Dictionary Lists
翻訳と辞書 辞書検索 [ 開発暫定版 ]
スポンサード リンク

Hyperacuity (scientific term) : ウィキペディア英語版
Hyperacuity (scientific term)
The sharpness of our senses is defined by the finest detail we can discriminate. Visual acuity is measured by the smallest letters that can be distinguished on a chart and is governed by the anatomical spacing of the mosaic of sensory elements on the retina. Yet spatial distinctions can be made on a finer scale still: misalignment of borders can be detected with a precision up to 10 times better than visual acuity. This
hyperacuity, transcending by far the size limits set by the retinal 'pixels', depends on sophisticated information processing in the brain.
==How does hyperacuity differ from traditional acuity?==
The best example of the distinction between acuity and hyperacuity comes from vision, for example when observing stars on a night sky. The first stage is the optical imaging of the outside world on the retina. Light impinges on the mosaic of receptor sense cells, rods and cones, which covers the retinal surface without gaps or overlap, just like the detecting pixels in the film plane of digital cameras. Each receptor accepts all the light reaching it but acts as a unit, representing a single location in visual space. This compartmentalization sets a limit to the decision whether an image came from a single or a double star (resolution). For a percept of separately articulated stars to emerge, the images of the two must be wide enough apart to leave at least one intervening pixel relatively unstimulated between them. This defines the resolution limit and the basis of visual acuity.
A quite different mechanism operates in hyperacuity, whose quintessential example and the one for which the word was initially coined, is vernier acuity: alignment of two edges or lines can be judged with a precision five or ten times better than acuity. A sophisticated circuitry in the brain identifies the location of a visual feature by assessing the “center of gravity” of the light over several receptors, a task that can be accomplished with much higher precision than the resolution limit set by the receptor spacing. In computer graphics the phrase “sub-pixel resolution” is sometimes used in discussions of anti-aliasing and geometrical superresolution. Though what is in fact involved is not ''resolution'' (is it one or two? – a ''qualitative'' distinction) but ''localization'' (exactly where? – a ''quantitative'' judgment) it captures the process. When an image spreads across several pixels, each with graded intensity response but only a single spatial value, the position of the image center can be located more exactly than the width of the pixel, much like the mean of a histogram can be calculated to a fraction of the bin width.
In the figure on the right, the retinal mosaic has superimposed on it, at top, the images of two stars at resolution limit when the intervening gap assures judgment that there are two stars and not a single elongated one. Shown below are the images of two separate short lines; the precision of the read-out of their location difference transcends the dimension of the mosaic elements.

抄文引用元・出典: フリー百科事典『 ウィキペディア(Wikipedia)
ウィキペディアで「Hyperacuity (scientific term)」の詳細全文を読む



スポンサード リンク
翻訳と辞書 : 翻訳のためのインターネットリソース

Copyright(C) kotoba.ne.jp 1997-2016. All Rights Reserved.